Home / Health / News Tools: Save | Print | E-mail | Most Read | Comment
Sleep selectively preserves emotional memories
Adjust font size:

A period of slumber helps the brain distinguish core emotions from background details

As poets, songwriters and authors have described, our memories range from misty water-colored recollections to vividly detailed images of the times of our lives.

Now, a study led by researchers at Beth Israel Deaconess Medical Center (BIDMC) and Boston College offers new insights into the specific components of emotional memories, suggesting that sleep plays a key role in determining what we remember – and what we forget.

Reported in the August 2008 issue of the journal Psychological Science, the findings show that a period of slumber helps the brain to selectively preserve and enhance those aspects of a memory that are of greatest emotional resonance, while at the same time diminishing the memory's neutral background details.

"This tells us that sleep's role in emotional memory preservation is more than just mechanistic," says the study's first author Jessica Payne, PhD, a Harvard University research fellow in the Division of Psychiatry at BIDMC. "In order to preserve what it deems most important, the brain makes a tradeoff, strengthening the memory's emotional core and obscuring its neutral background."

Previous studies have established the key role that sleep plays in procedural memory, demonstrating that the consolidation of procedural skills (such as typing or playing the piano) is greatly enhanced following a period of sleep.

But sleep's importance in the development of episodic memories – in particular, those with emotional resonance– has been less clear.

"Emotional memories usually contain highly charged elements – for example, the car that sideswiped us on the ride home – along with other elements that are only tangentially related to the emotion, such as the name of the street we were traveling on or what store we'd just passed," explains study author Elizabeth Kensinger, PhD, an Assistant Professor in the College of Arts and Sciences at Boston College. "We were interested in examining whether sleep would affect memory for all of these elements equally, or whether sleep might allow some of the event features to decay at a faster rate than others."

The authors tested 88 college students. Study participants were shown scenes that depicted either neutral subjects on a neutral background (a car parked on a street in front of shops) or negatively arousing subjects on a neutral background (a badly crashed car parked on a similar street). The participants were then tested separately on their memories of both the central objects in the pictures and the backgrounds in the scenes. In this way, memory could be compared for the emotional aspects of a scene (the crashed car) versus the non-emotional aspects of the scene (the street on which the car had crashed.)

Subjects were divided into three groups. The first group underwent memory testing after 12 hours spent awake during the daytime; the second group was tested after 12 nighttime hours, including their normal period of nighttime sleep; and the third baseline group was tested 30 minutes after viewing the images, in either the morning or evening.

"Our results revealed that the study subjects who stayed awake all day largely forgot the entire negative scene [they had seen], with their memories of both the central objects and the backgrounds decaying at similar rates," says Payne. But, she adds, among the individuals who were tested after a period of sleep, memory recall for the central negative objects (i.e. the smashed car) was preserved in detail.

"After an evening of sleep, the subjects remembered the emotional items [smashed car] as accurately as the subjects whose memories had been tested only 30 minutes after looking at the scenes," explains Kensinger. "By contrast, sleep did little to preserve memory for the backgrounds [i.e. street scenes] and so memory for those elements reached a comparably low level after a night of sleep as it did after a day spent awake."

"This is consistent with the possibility that the individual components of emotional memory become 'unbound' during sleep," adds Payne, explaining that "unbinding" enables the sleeping brain to selectively preserve only that information which it calculates to be most salient and worthy of remembering. A real-world example of this tradeoff, she adds, is the "weapon focus effect" in which crime victims vividly remember an assailant's weapon, but have little memory for other important aspects of the crime scene. Traumatic memories, such as the flashbacks experienced among individuals with post-traumatic stress disorder, can demonstrate similar disparities, with some aspects of an experience seemingly engraved in memory while other details are erased.

"Sleep is a smart, sophisticated process," adds Payne. "You might say that sleep is actually working at night to decide what memories to hold on to and what to let go of."

(Agencies August 14, 2008)

Tools: Save | Print | E-mail | Most Read
Comment
Pet Name
Anonymous
China Archives
Related >>
Most Viewed >>
- Fit and fat: Study shows it's possible
- Difference between married, unmarried narrowed
- UNICEF rallies youths to raise HIV/AIDS awareness
- Potatoes may hold key to Alzheimer's treatment
- Easy does it - Don't sit too long on your butt
主站蜘蛛池模板: 熟妇人妻久久中文字幕| 黑人巨鞭大战欧美肥妇| 岳的大白屁股光溜溜| 久久亚洲色一区二区三区| 欧美一区二区三区高清不卡tv| 人人做人人爽人人爱| 精品久久久久久久无码| 国产69精品久久久久妇女| 鲁丝丝国产一区二区| 日韩爽爽视频爽爽| 亚洲人成77777在线播放网站 | 国产亚洲欧美日韩在线观看一区二区| 日本高清在线免费| 国产精品无码久久久久久| 久久99精品久久久久麻豆| 日韩视频在线观看| 人人妻人人澡人人爽曰本| 精品亚洲综合久久中文字幕| 国产欧美在线不卡| 香蕉伊思人在线精品| 国产高清一区二区三区视频| 99这里只有精品| 天天操天天射天天爽| 一区二区三区中文字幕| 成人在线免费观看网站| 中文字幕精品一区| 日日摸日日碰夜夜爽亚洲| 久久久无码人妻精品无码| 日本韩国在线视频| 亚洲熟妇无码av在线播放| 狠狠色噜噜狠狠狠合久| 国产东北老头老太露脸| 韩国免费特一级毛片| 国产大陆亚洲精品国产| 91香蕉成人免费网站| 国产手机精品一区二区| 99精品视频在线视频免费观看| 女人张开腿让男人捅| vvvv99日韩精品亚洲| 日本乱子伦xxxx| 久久久精品午夜免费不卡|